Stereotypes suggest that women can talk for hours
about their feelings, while men share less about their emotions. Though this typecast
may be a prevalent societal perception, in reality the opposite may be true, particularly
in settings where men and women are publically speaking within a group.
Observing different public discourse settings may provide insight into women’s
verbal participation, specifically in more formal public situations such as
political settings. This may also lead to a better understanding of how women
participate in politics. Mendelberg and Karpowitz suggest that women make
policy differences only when they have true parity with men. [1] It is
essential that women participate; and when women participate less than men, women
are by default, less represented.
Hypothesis
In order for us to know how to make sure women are
better represented, we must first see if women truly do participate less in
public settings. I hypothesized that women would participate less frequently
than men in classroom, church, and political settings. Research by the UN
suggested that women with decision-making authority succeeded in introducing
changes to improve the lives of many women. [2] This convinced more women in
the general public to participate because they saw that women could make real
policy changes. [2] Because of this, I also speculated that when the
discussions were led by women, more women would participate.
In addition to how often women and men participated,
I wanted to look at the kind of comments they made. Sociologists Schau and
Meierding suggested that women use hedging comments more than men, which are
comments like “this might be wrong” or “I’m not sure but…” [3] This has even
been seen in professional settings, where women use hedging even when dealing with someone who is in a lower
power position than herself. [3] Lawless and Fox found that women
believe that they know a lot less about policy issues and also believe that
they are less qualified [4]. Because of this research, I hypothesized that
women would not only participate less, but when they did participate, they
would hedge their comments.
Observation
Methods
I observed participation in different settings where
discourse takes place: two discussion-based classes, Sunday school, BYU College
Democrats, and BYU College Republicans. I kept track of the number of times
that men spoke and the number of times that women spoke in all of these
settings over the course of two meetings.
I originally hypothesized that when the discussion
was led by a woman, women in the group would participate more. In order to
observe this, I looked at every setting with both a woman leading the
discussion and a man leading the discussion. First, I looked at two Journalism classes
of similar size that were discussion-based. I also looked at two Sunday school
classes with the same lesson content. In both of these cases, I observed one
class where a man taught and another where a woman taught. Finally, I also
attended a BYU College Democrat’s meeting which was led by two women, and a BYU
College Republican’s meeting which was led by two men. Additionally, I took note of the kind of comments
that were made in these settings by both men and women. Anytime a man or woman
added a hedging comment to their statement, I would write their exact words.
After I observed these participation based meetings,
I found that women spoke less in every single setting. Furthermore, when women
did speak, they would often hedge their comments.
In the journalism classes,
women participated less than men. Details of this participation is seen in
Table 1. Additionally, I heard hedging comments from women in both of these
classes. In the journalism class with a woman teacher, four of the women gave
hedging comments. Further, these hedging comments took up about half of the
time that they spent talking. The men in the class did not use any hedging
comments. In the
journalism class with
a man teaching, all of the girls who spoke used hedging comments. The men never made hedged comments.
In Sunday school, I observed similar things as I did
in the classrooms. In both cases, women spoke much less than men. The
exact observations can be seen in Table 2. I further noticed that there were
more hedging comments in the church setting than any other setting. Every woman
who spoke added a hedging comment. At church, the woman’s hedging comments were
often things like “I understand that I do not know everything because I do not
have the priesthood..." In the church setting, women suggested that they knew less than men so
they felt they could not fully contribute to the class.
One thing that I found interesting is that no hedging
comments were made from men or women in either of these political settings. One
suggestion for this finding may be that people who came to these meetings did
so because they were interested in and already had a background knowledge of
money and politics, so they felt confident enough in their knowledge that they
did not feel the need to excuse themselves before making comments.
Conclusion
Overall, I found that women participated much less
than men. When women did participate, they gave hedging comments to excuse
their words. Although these findings may be telling of the gender gap in participation,
I found no evidence as to why this may be. I originally suggested that perhaps
when women lead the conversation, women are more likely to participate.
However, my observations did not support that hypothesis. There has been past
research (which I discussed above) which suggests that when there are women
leading discussions, women participate more. Since my findings were not consistent
with those, limitations in my research that should be noted. I only observed meetings
that were filled with BYU students, and BYU students are not likely to be
completely representative of the entire public. Mormon culture also tends to
create more submissive women, which may be why women participated less. In the
BYU College Democrats and BYU College Republican meetings, the topic was about
money in politics, which may be inherently more interesting for men, which led
them to participate more. If the topic were about a policy issue that women
found more interesting, such as the gender wage gap, women may have
participated more.
For further research, it might be interesting to
look at the ratio between men and women in the group to see if more women in a
group means more participation from women in this group. Instead of just
observing, I may want to run experiments setting up specific situations, such
as having a women leader and man leader. If I run my own experiment, I would be
able to hold all other things constant and perhaps receive different results. It
might also be telling to do more qualitative research and ask women how they
feel in these group settings to perhaps understand more why it is that they
participate less and why they feel the need to add hedging comments.
[3] Jan
Frankel and Nina Meierding. 2007. Negotiating
Like a Woman - How Gender Impacts Communication between the Sexes. Print.
Journal of Mediating.
No comments:
Post a Comment